Schmidt adaptive nf

Biomed Tech 2012; 57 (Suppl. 1) © 2012 by Walter de Gruyter · Berlin · Boston. DOI 10.1515/bmt-2012-4444 Patient Adapti...

0 downloads 102 Views 308KB Size
Biomed Tech 2012; 57 (Suppl. 1) © 2012 by Walter de Gruyter · Berlin · Boston. DOI 10.1515/bmt-2012-4444

Patient Adaptive Neurofeedback for ADHD Therapy Tino Schmidt (Department of Psychology, TU Dresden), Dresden, Germany, [email protected]; Dietmar Henrich (Dept. of Biomedical Engineering, Lausitz University of Applied Sciences, Senftenberg, Germany, [email protected]); Keywords: ADHD, externalized feedback, EEG, hardware-in the-loop

Abstract An EEG-Based audiovisual stimulation system was tested on a group of 32 ADHD patients. The efficacy was compared in a pre- and post analysis over a treatment period of 3 month with a control group and a group of patients with an EEGbased neurofeedback therapy. The number of impulsivity errors in a stop-signal paradigm at several distinct times during and after the therapy was used as a quality marker. The main feature of this method uses real-time-EEG data derived parameters for a controlled manipulation of EEG patterns with persistently strong audio-visual stimuli. It is based upon a direct feedback of changes in short-term coherences in the context of central nervous stimulus processing to control the stimulating frequency. The real-time EEG processing is done on the basis of time-dependent coherence analysis of the theta and beta band.

1

Introduction

1.1

ADHD

ADHD is with 10% the most prevalent psychic disorder in children and adolescents [1,2]. The affected persons don’t possess the cognitive ability to follow short conversations and act permanently goal-oriented. Therefor is their school and work performance under par even with an IQ above average. The permanent experience of failure results in most of the affected people in further psychic problems – typically anxiety disorders, depression and drug abuse [3]. They are difficult to treat at the same time, because they cannot follow in an appropriate way to the therapeutic sessions due to their lack of attention. Therefor first their attention deficit needs to be treated before secondary diseases can be cured. ADHD manifests itself on a neurophysiological scale as a striatofrontal dysfunction [4,5]. A reduced metabolic rate is proven in the frontal cortex [6] and on the right side of the frontal-striatal system [7]. The affected areas show a dysfunction of the dopaminergic and noradrenergic transmitters, who are responsible for a precise regulation of the behavioral reactions [8]. At the moment ADHD must be considered as almost not treatable. Only the symptoms can be pharmaceutically treated (mostly via Methylphenidat, more seldom via Antidepressiva) by temporally reducing the dopaminergic resp. noradrenergic transmitter balances. Therefore the medical treatment of the symptoms works only as long as medicine is active. After which the previously achieved improvements vanish [9,10]. After the effect of the medication declines which is already a couple of hours later the ADHD symptoms return back and occasionally even enhanced [11,12] (Esser, 2002; Lubar, 2003). This implies that the administration of drugs doesn’t lead to a learning process in the children or an behavioral change. Pelham & Gnagy [13] came to the conclusion that so far in none of the studies with a medical

treatment, ADHD adolescents have had any persistent long-term benefit of a social appropriate behavior. Other psychotherapeutic measures like behavioral or cognitive therapy show a high drop out rate and/or only shortterm improvements [14] Regarding the efficiency of neurofeedback contradicting results are shown [15,16,17]. Only the individually tuned combined treatment of several methods led to sustained improvement of the symptoms [18,13,19]. Due economical unfavorable factors these methods didn’t succeed in practice [20].

1.2.

Neurofeedback versus Stimulation

The theoretical background of the application of neurofeedback methods is derived from numerous studies implying that ADHD patients show a deviating EEG pattern in multiple ways compared to normal people who are able to concentrate. Often it is assumed that ADHD patients show an increased theta-band EEG pattern (4-8 HZ) whereas the beta-band in the EEG (12-30 Hz) is reduced, although not all treatments rely on this assumption. [21,22,23]. With neurofeedback the patients learn to control EEG pattern via the feedback information of some physiologic data., which normally is not voluntarily controlled. This requires however a strong inner sensibility and concentration [24]. Exactly here lies the main reason for the high drop out rates and lacking of efficiency of non-medical treatments. because as well the therapeutic sessions as the neurofeedback training requires a minimal set of concentration ability, but this is what is actually missing. At this point several stimulation methods start operating to externally influence the EEG- patterns. The general effect of persistent audio-visual stimuli on the EEG is well known since the early days of EEG, when the alpha-band could be influenced via well targeted visual stimuli [25,26] . In principal the procedure is to expose the patient with a series of light and/or acoustical impulses and to observe the subjective-physiological or EEG-pattern changes. The stimulation is performed within a fixed time

655

Biomed Tech 2012; 57 (Suppl. 1) © 2012 by Walter de Gruyter · Berlin · Boston. DOI 10.1515/bmt-2012-4444

period with a predetermined frequency [27], where the stimuli should be intense enough to pass the thalamus [28]. Although the positive effect of persistent audio-visual stimuli e.g. for the treatment of migrane [29],chronic pain [30] or depression [31] could be shown, the mechanism could not be target controlled satisfactory. Partly it is possible to affect certain frequency bands, but not for all frequency bands as well [32]. Furthermore it is unclear how relevant the affected frequency band is for the goal in mind. Stimulation methods are not patient adaptive – here is their main disadvantage. At a first glance neurofeedback and brainwaveentrainment methods seem to be based on the same principle to transform a dysfunctional EEG pattern into a normal one. The impression is that with neurofeedback the patient actively influence this transformation and with stimuli methods this is achieved passively via external stimuli. The underlying neural mechanism remains unclear, it seems to be sure that a bigger entity of the global neural system must be altered in order for the brain to accomplish such a transformation out of itself, even if is merely an external conditioning [33]. This disadvantage of not being patient adaptive explains why brainwave entrainment methods are less efficient compared to feedback methods as would be expected., because they don’t rely on a concentration and learning ability. Some studies indeed confirm the effectiveness of entrainment for ADHD [34,35], but not in all cognitive areas [30,36].

2

Methods

2.1.

Hardware in the loop - Therapy

Simplified the core of our proposed therapeutic method consists of a patient-adaptive stimulation process wrapped around a neurofeedback method. Since ADHD patients show more theta-band (4-8 Hz) and less beta-band (12-30 Hz) brain activity it would be obvious to stimulate the beta-band. Astonishingly this hardly leads to an improvement of the symptoms. It is to be assumed that this theta over beta ratio mismatch is by itself rather a symptom of more fundamental underlying neuronal processes. It was shown that this ratio mismatch of the two frequency components is due to a lack in synchronization of cognitive feedback loops [37,38] (Barry et al., 2002; Chabot & Serfontein, 1996) Therefore ADHD patients suffer from the ability to adapt action pursuing and action controlling resp. correcting process to each other. An Effective entrainment method should therefore influence the synchrony of the relevant feedback loops and the resulting frequency components. A special entrainment method was developed that addresses the synchrony of the disturbing feedback loops. It is based upon a time-variant coherence analysis developed in our group, which allows to localize the sources and the frequency components involved in ADHD [39]. Based on this procedure it is possible via the changes of the evoked

potentials to detect ”online” the disturbing frequency components while performing an audiovisual stimuli with realtime patient adaptive neuronal feedback [40] which otherwise would be only possible within the brain itself during neurofeedback. This method is therefore called „hardware- in –the –loop“-like. A 32 channel EEG system (10/20-system) delivers signals being first amplified (EEG 8, Contact Precision Instruments) filtered (high-pass: 0.01 Hz, low- pass: 40 Hz, time constant: 16 s) digitized and transferred to a PC, where the incoming data are real-time processed to detect short term coherences. If such a coherence is detected for at least 60ms at two electrode positions in different brain hemispheres then a stimulation frequency is applied in the same direction as the previously detected coherence.

2.2.

Proof of Efficiency

To test the process 32 patients were randomly selected for the therapy sessions when they fulfilled to following criteria: •DSM-IV Criteria for ADHD, ADS or HD [41] • age: 9 – 15 years • IQ > 80, using the Hamburg-Wechsler-IQ-test for children (HAWIK-III) [42] • no other neurological or psychological disorder The sample comprised 3 girls and 29 boys (age average=11,2; SD=1,53). The control group consisted of 26 patients waiting for a therapy during the 3 month testing period. (23 boys, age average=10,6; SD=1,73). As a further control group we used 18 patients (16 boys; age average=11,7; SD=1,32), which underwent a neurofeedback training where the theta/beta ratio was an input parameter. The progress was controlled using two subsets of tests from the TAP (Testbattery for Attentioncontrol, Version 1.7, [43]: the test subsets Go/NoGo and reaction change. In both subsets the ADHD children made more errors compared to the control group and showed a higher variability in reaction times, so that 90% of the children could be classified correctly having ADHD (positive predictive validity=0. 86 / negative predictive validity=0.93) [44]

3

Results

The progress was controlled using two subsets of tests from the TAP (Testbattery for Attentioncontrol, Version 1.7, [43]): the test subsets Go/NoGo and reaction change. In both subsets the ADHD children made more errors compared to the control group and showed a higher variability in reaction times, so that 90% of the children could be classified correctly having ADHD (positive predictive validity=0. 86 / negative predictive validity=0.93)[44]. Images 1 and 2 show the under and above average results of the subsets before the actual training sessions, 3 measurements during the sessions period of 3 month and 1 posttraining measurements after 1 month resp for all three groups. The evaluation of the complete test was done all common parameters as well as for the change rates (average, standard deviation).

656

Biomed Tech 2012; 57 (Suppl. 1) © 2012 by Walter de Gruyter · Berlin · Boston. DOI 10.1515/bmt-2012-4444

4

TAT / Reactiontask, < 25

The lacking efficiency of the neurofeedback method could be due to the fact that 6 out of 11 patients prematurely stopped the treatment. Therefore the significance is beforehand hampered. Exactly the opposite was true in the case of the “hardware-in-the-loop” group. It could be concluded that a method based upon external EEG feedback with adaptive stimulation exactly that neuronal feedback systems could be excited which are relevant for the concentration process, which could otherwise only by addressed by training intense neurofeedback techniques.

6 Control Neurofeedback Hardware-in-the-loop

normed Test Results

5

4

3

2

5

1

0

screening

1st Month

2nd Month

3rd Month

2nd post

Timeline of Therapy

Image 1 Number of under-averaged (PR<25) test parameter of the Go/NoGo subset-test

TAT / Reactiontask, > 75 6

Col 10 Col 12 Col 14

normed Test Results

5

4

3

2

1

0

screening

1st Month

2nd Month

3rd Month

Conclusions

2nd post

Timeline of Therapy

Image 2 Number of above average (PR>75) test parameter of the Go/NoGo subset-test ANOVA analysis with measurement repetition, measurement time und experimental group resulted only in the „hardware-in-the –loop“ group a significant reduction in the rate change of the subset group „reaction change“ of the under average test parameter (pre versus post =0.000) and a significant increase of the above average test parameter(p=0.000). The number of under 25% and above 75% test criteria in the Go/NoGo subtest also changed only in the same group significantly (p = .048 resp.. .34). A comparison of the 3 groups regarding the interaction of the parameters didn’t show any significant effects (p=.472 resp. p =0.276).

References

[1] Analog Devices: Analog Design Seminar. München: Analog Devices GmbH, 1989 [1] Lee, S. W. (1991) Biofeedback as a treatment for childhood hyperactivity: A critical review of the literature. Psychological Report, 68 (1), 163 – 192. [2] Scahill, L., & Schwab- Stone, M. (2000). Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9 (3), 541 – 555. [3] Steinhausen, H.C. (Hrsg.) (1995). Hyperkinetische Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. [4] Dougherty et al. (1999) Dopamine transporter density in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In: Lancet 354 2132-2133. [5] Spencer T.J. et al. (2005) In Vivo Neuroreceptor Imaging in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Focus on The Dopamine Transporter, Biological Psychiatry, 57 (11), 1293-1300. [6] Lou, H., Henriksen, L., & Bruhn, P. (1984). Focal cerebral hypo-perfusion in children with dysphasia and/ or attention deficit disorder. Archives of Neurology, 41: 825 – 829. [7] Heilman, K.M., Voeller, K., & Nadeu, S.E. (1991). A possible pathophysiologi-cal substrate of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, 6 (Suppl), 74 – 79. [8] Fuchs, T., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., Gruzelier, J.H., & Kaiser, J. (2003). Neurofeedback treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children: A comparison with methylphenidate. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 28 (1), 1 – 12. [9] Jensen, P.S., Garcia, J.A., Glied, S., Crowe, M., Foster, M., Schlander, M., Hinshow, S., Vitiello, B., Arnold, L. E., Elliott, G., Hechtman, L., Newcorn, J.H., Pelham, W.E., Swanson, J., & Wells, K. (2005). Costeffectiveness of ADHD treatments: Findings from the multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162 (9), 1628 – 1636 [10] Pelham, W.E., Wheeler, T., & Chronis, A. (1998). Empirically supported psychosocial treatments for at-

657

Biomed Tech 2012; 57 (Suppl. 1) © 2012 by Walter de Gruyter · Berlin · Boston. DOI 10.1515/bmt-2012-4444

tention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27 (2), 190 – 205. [11] Esser, G. (2002). Lehrbuch der klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag. [12] Lubar, J.F. (2003). Neurofeedback for the management of attention deficit disorders. In M.S. Schwartz & F. Andrasik (Eds.), Biofeedback: A practitioner’s guide (3rd ed.), (409 – 437), New York: Guilford Press. [13]Pelham & Gnagy [] (1999) [14] Döpfner, M. (2002). Hyperkinetische Störungen. In: G. Esser (Hrsg.), Lehrbuch der klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters (172 – 196). Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag. [15] Lee, S. W. (1991) Biofeedback as a treatment for childhood hyperactivity: A critical review of the literature. Psychological Report, 68 (1), 163 – 192. [16] Kaiser, D.A., & Othmer, S. (2000). Effect of neurofeedback on variables of attention in a large multicenter trial. Journal of Neurotherapy, 4: 5 – 15. [17] DeBeus R., Ball J.D., deBeus M.E., & Herrington, R. (2003). Attention training with ADHD children: Preliminary findings in a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Presented at the Annual conference of the international society for neuronal regulation, Houston. [18]Saile, 1996; [19] Rowland, Lesesne & Abramowitz, 2002 [20] Jensen, P.S. (2002). ADHD comorbidity findings from the MTA study: New diagnostic subtypes and their optimal treatments. In J.E. Helzer & J. James (Eds.) Defining psychopathology in the 21st century: DSM- V and beyond. Washington, DC, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc [21] Lazzaro, I., Gordon, E., Whitmont, S., Plahn, M., Li, W., Clarke, S., Dosen, A., & Meares, R. (1998). Quantified EEG activity in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Electroencephalography, 29 (1), 37 – 42. [22] Monastra, V.J., Endicott, N.Y., Lubar, J.F., Linden, M., VanDeusen, P., Green, G., Wing, W., Phillips, A., & Fenger, T.N. (1999). Assessing attention deficit hyperactivity disorder via quantitative electroencephalography: An initial validation study. Neuropsychology, 13 (3), 424 – 433. [23] Bresnahan, S.M., & Barry, R.J. (2002). Specificity of quantitative EEG analysis in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 112 (2), 133 – 144. [24] Masterpasqua, F., & Healey, K.N. (2003). Neurofeedback in psychological practice. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34 (6), 652 – 656. [25] Bartley, S. (1934) Relation of intensity and duration of brief retinal stimulation by light to the electrical response to the optic cortex of the rabbit. American Journal of Physiology, 108: 397-408. [26] Durup, G. & Fessard, A. (1935) L'electroencephalogramme de l'homme (the human electroencephalogram). Annale Psychologie, 36: 1-32.

[27] Collura, T. & Siever, D. (2009). Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback (2nd Ed.), chapter Audiovisual entrainment in relation to mental health and EEG., pp. 155-183. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [28] Siever, D. (2007) Handbook of Neurofeedback: Dynamics and Clinical Applications, chapter Audiovisual entrainment: history, physiology, and clinical studies., pages 155-183. Binghamton, NY: The Haworth Medical Press. [29] Anderson, D. (1989) The treatment of migraine with variable frequency photic stimulation. Headache, 29:154-155. [30] Huang, T.L. & Charyton, C. (2008) A comprehensive review of the psychological effects of brainwave entrainment. Altern Ther Health Med, 14(5):38-50. [31] Boersma, F.J. & Gagnon, С. (1992). The use of repetitive audio-visual entrainment in the management of chronic pain. Medical Hypnoanalysis Journal, 7:462-468. [32] Lubar J., Rasey H., Brim S., Blackburn J. & Frederick, J. (1999) Effects of 18.5 Hz audiovisual stimulation on eeg amplitude at the vertex. Journal of Neurotherapy, 3(3):23-27. [33] Sterman, M.B. (1996). Physiological origins and functional correlates of EEG rhythmic activities: Implications for self-regulation. Biofeedback and Self Regulation, 21 (1), 3 – 33. [34] Patrick, J. G. (1996). Improved Neuronal Regulation in ADHD: A Application of Fifteen Sessions of Photic-­‐ Driven EEG. Neurotherapy. J Neurother, 1: 27– 36. [35] Joyce, M. & Siever, D. (2000) Audio-visual entrainment program as a treatment for behavior disorders in a school setting. Journal of Neurotherapy, 4(2):9-15. [36] Barry,R.A., Clarke,A.R., McCarthy,R., & Selikowitz,M. (2002). EEG coherence in attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparative study of two DSM-IV types. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 579-585. [37] Chabot,J.R., & Serfontein,G. (1996). Quantitative Electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. [Abstract]. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 951- 963. [38] Volke, H. J. Dettmar, P. Richter, P. Rudolf, M. Buhss, U. (1999) Evoced coherences of EEG in mental load: An investigation in chess players. Zeitschr. für Psychologie, 207 (2), 233-262. [39] Volke, H.-J. Dettmar, P.; Richter, P.; Rudolf, M. & Buhss, U (2002) On-coupling and off-coupling of neocortical areas in chess experts EEG coherence measures J. Psychophysiol. 16, 23–36. [40] Wittchen,H.U. (1989). Diagnostisches und Statistisches Manual Psychischer Störungen DSM-III-R. Weinheim: Beltz. [41] Tewes,U., Rossman,P., & Schallberger,U. (1999). Hamburg Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder - Dritte Auflage (HAWIK III). Bern: Huber. [42] Zimmermann,P., & Fimm,B. (2002). Testbatterie zur

658